Researchers from Johns Hopkins University have found that one brain-training method often used in scientific studies can help to improve working memory.
Our working memory is what we use on a day-to-day basis, especially at school or in a work context. It refers to our ability to pick up new information and adapt our responses accordingly, over brief periods of time.
Working memory is a key part of the learning process, and it has been linked to literacy and numeracy abilities. It is no surprise, then, that scientists have been investigating whether there are any “recipes for success” when it comes to improving working memory.
So far, studies have not conclusively pinpointed an effective method of training the brain to boost its cognitive abilities. But now, researchers based at Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, MD, have used a fresh approach to test the effectiveness of different brain-training methods.
They have found, for the first time, that one exercise can significantly enhance working memory.
“People say cognitive training either works or doesn’t work. We showed that it matters what kind of training you’re doing. This one task seems to show the most consistent results and the most impact on performance and should be the one we focus on if we’re interested in improving cognition through training.”
Lead study author Dr. Kara J. Blacker
The researchers’ findings were published in the Journal of Cognitive Enhancement earlier this week.
Brain training through memory tasks
Dr. Blacker and colleagues hypothesized that previous studies may not have focused on the most effective brain-training tasks when attempting to address the question, “Can we improve some of our cognitive abilities?”
Thus, in their own study, the researchers compared two main types of brain-training activity. They also measured the participants’ brain activity both before and after they had engaged in these exercises, using electroencephalograms (EEG).
The team recruited 136 young adults (of whom 44 were male), who were then split into three groups. All the participants were subjected to the same tests at baseline, in order to ascertain working memory abilities, attention, and intelligence levels. Brain activity was also measured by EEG at baseline.
Two types of brain-training exercise were tackled in this study: “dual n-back” and “complex span.”
Dual n-back training involves receiving visuospatial and auditory information simultaneously. In this case, as explained in the paper, “[p]articipants saw a blue square on a gray background in one of eight spatial locations on a computer screen and at the same time heard one of eight letter sounds.”
The information was continuously updated, and they were asked to remember sequences that they were exposed to 1 minute ago, 2 minutes ago, or a few minutes back as the difficulty of the task increased.
In the other brain-training exercise, complex span, “[t]he participant’s task was to remember the order and locations of red squares presented in a 4 x 4 grid.” This exercise, however, did not require constantly updating the “store” of information they had just received.
‘Sequencing and updating’ task effective
Split up into three groups, the participants had to do the dual n-back task, the complex span task, or a control task. They engaged in these tasks for a month, spread across 5 days per week for a total of 30 minutes each time.
After the final EEG scans, the scientists found that the working memory of participants who had engaged in the dual n-back task had improved by 30 percent — which is almost twice as much as the improvement shown in participants who had been assigned the complex span exercise.
The same participants also displayed changes in the activity of the prefrontal cortex, which is a brain region highly implicated in attention and memory.
“The findings suggest that this particular task [dual n-back] is changing something about the brain. There’s something about sequencing and updating that really taps into the things that only the pre-frontal cortex can do, the real-world problem-solving tasks,” explains study co-author Prof. Susan Courtney.
Following this study, the researchers are interested in understanding what makes this particular task an effective exercise for enhancing working memory. They would also like to adapt this exercise to make it more consumer-oriented, or marketable in a clinical context.
“The biggest lesson here was that — yes — intensive training strengthens cognition and the brain, but we still don’t understand why and how. We can’t just jump onto a video game and expect that’s going to cure all of our cognitive problems. We need more targeted interventions,” comments Prof. Courtney.